Page:Sm all cc.pdf/20

From Wikisource
Jump to navigation Jump to search
This page has been proofread, but needs to be validated.
17

“Truth comes out of error more readily than out of confusion.” [Francis Bacon, 1620]

“The man who makes no mistakes does not usually make anything.” [Phelps, 1899]

Incorrect but intriguing hypotheses can be valuable, because the investigations that they inspire may lead to a discovery or at least show the way toward a better hypothesis (Chapter 7). Humphrey Davy [1840] said, “The most important of my discoveries have been suggested by my failures.” More rarely, incorrect evidence can inspire a fruitful hypothesis: Eldredge and Gould’s [1972] seminal reinterpretation of Darwinian evolution as punctuated equilibrium initially was based on inappropriate examples [Brown, 1987].

Errors are most likely to be detected upon first exposure. Once overlooked, they become almost invisible. For example, if an erroneous hypothesis passes initial tests and is accepted, it becomes remarkably immune to overthrow.

“One definition of an expert is a person who knows all the possible mistakes and how to avoid them. But when we say that people are ‘wise’ it’s not usually because they’ve made every kind of mistake there is to make (and learned from them), but because they have stored up a lot of simulated scenarios, because their accumulated quality judgments (whether acted upon or not) have made them particularly effective in appraising a novel scenario and advising on a course of action.” [Calvin, 1986]

Errors arise unavoidably: unrecognized variations in experimental conditions generate both socalled ‘random’ errors and systematic errors. The researcher needs to know the possible effects of errors on experimental data, in order to judge whether or not to place any confidence in resulting conclusions. Without knowledge of the errors, one cannot compare an experimental result to a theoretical prediction, compare two experimental results, or evaluate whether an apparent correlation is real. In short, the data are nearly useless.

Precision > Accuracy > Reliability

The terms precision, accuracy, reliability, confidence, and replicatability are used interchangeably by most non-scientists and are even listed by many dictionaries as largely synonymous. In their scientific usage, however, these terms have specific and important distinctions.

Errors affect the precision and accuracy of measurements. Precision is a measure of the scatter, dispersion, or replicatability of the measurements. Low-precision, or high-scatter, measurements are sometimes referred to as noisy data. Smaller average difference between repeat (replicate) measurements means higher precision. For example, if we measure a sheet of paper several times with a ruler, we might get measurements such as 10.9", 11.0", 10.9", and 11.1". If we used a micrometer instead, we might get measurements such as 10.97", 10.96", 10.98", and 10.97". Our estimates show random variation regardless of the measuring device, but the micrometer gives a more precise measurement than does the ruler. If our ruler or micrometer is poorly made, it may yield measurements that are consistently offset, or systematically biased, from the true lengths. Accuracy is the extent to which the measurements are a reliable estimate of the ‘true’ value. Both random errors and systematic biases reduce accuracy.